Реферат

Реферат на тему Intellectual Development Of Children Essay Research Paper

Работа добавлена на сайт bukvasha.net: 2015-06-14

Поможем написать учебную работу

Если у вас возникли сложности с курсовой, контрольной, дипломной, рефератом, отчетом по практике, научно-исследовательской и любой другой работой - мы готовы помочь.

Предоплата всего

от 25%

Подписываем

договор

Выберите тип работы:

Скидка 25% при заказе до 11.11.2024


Intellectual Development Of Children Essay, Research Paper

In two separate issues of ?Time? magazine, the intellectual development of

infants and preschoolers was analyzed with contrasting viewpoints regarding the

development of their brains and the views regarding how best to encourage the

cognitive abilities of these young children. In the earlier issue, dated

February 3, 1997, the special report consisting of two articles titled

?Fertile Minds? and ?The Day-Care Dilemma? the theories of Jean

Piaget?s cognitive-development are supported. In the latter issue, dated

October 19, 1998, the special report titled ?How to Make a Better Student?

focused on refuting the theories supported in the earlier issue of this

magazine. Understanding the influence of Piaget?s and other?s views on

intellectual development of young children on the contrasting views of this

topic and how it reflects contemporary opinions on how young children should be

raised is the focus of this paper. Hopefully, these contrasting articles will

provide a more holistic understanding of Piagetian theory and its application to

real-life situations. I. Children’s Intellectual Development: Preoperations By

the age of 3 and 4 years old, children have attained what Piaget called

functions or "preoperations" that enable young children to perform a

number of feats far beyond the capabilities of infants (Piaget, 1950). Infants

concentrate on constructing a world of permanent objects. Once constructed,

these objects will be known to exist even when they are no longer present to the

infant’s senses. Preschool children, in contrast, are constructing a world of

qualities and properties that different objects share in common. They are

beginning to identify and name colors, shapes, textures, density, and so on. At

this stage, children are beginning to understand same and different as these

terms refer to properties. At the same time, it is necessary to recognize that

these classes are formed only on the basis of perceptual attributes such as

color and form and not on the basis of any quantitative characteristics.

Moreover, although children can name and identify members of different classes

cow, dog, or car, they cannot as yet operate on these categories in a systematic

way. That is to say they cannot logically add categories and recognize that

cats, dogs, and cows are all animals. Nor can they logically multiply classes

and appreciate that a cat is both a cat and an animal at the same time. In

short, the one-many or quantitative dimension of classes escapes young children.

Only when they have attained the concrete operations of childhood (age 6 to 7

years) will they begin to be able to coordinate sameness and difference and

arrive at the notion of a unit that is basic to all quantitative thinking. A

unit, for example the number 3, is at once like every other number in that it is

a number but also different in that it is the only number that comes after 2 and

before 4. Once children have a notion of a unit, they can engage in numerical as

well as logical addition and multiplication (Gesell, 1949). The young child’s

limitation with respect to operating on classes is most evident when we ask them

to define a word. Young children routinely define words by describing their

functions; an apple is to eat; a bike is to ride. Only when they attain concrete

operations at about the age of 6 or 7 years will they begin to define terms by

nesting them in higher order classes, where an apple is a fruit, and a bike has

wheels–you go places with it. Occasionally young children may define a word by

placing it within a broader context, but this is often an anticipation of later

intellectual achievement, not a true reflection of the young child’s competence

(Carey, 1989). In the ?Fertile Minds? and ?The Day-Care Dilemma?

articles, neuroscientific evidence is used to comply with Piagetian theory of

preoperational stages of development. The article describes in depth how the

rapidly proliferating brain cells at birth make connections that shape a

lifetime of experience. Thus, the author of this article points at that the

first three to four years of development are critical and that ?there is an

urgent need . . . for preschool programs designed to boost the brain power of

youngsters? (Nash, 1997, p.51) This argument is supported by hard,

quantifiable evidence in the form of PET scans. Furthermore, the use of

Piagetian tools to be incorporated in the education of toddlers in day-cares is

stated to be a means that society can use to promote the intellectual

development of young children who live in conditions that are a threat to their

brain development. In ?How to Make a Better Student,? Craig Ramey, a

cognitive neuroscientist, states that PET scans have fueled unwarranted views of

brain development of young children and a preoccupation with the cognitive

development of their babies (Cole, 1998). The use of high contrast toys and

musical cribs, for example, are frowned upon in this later article. Rather, this

article stresses the importance of less input and increased protection. II.

Variability of Individual Growth Rates In discussing young children’s

intellectual growth and abilities, it is difficult to overemphasize the wide

range of normal variability in the age at which they attain their new mental

powers. The article ?Fertile Minds? seems to downplay this variability by

using scientific evidence of PET scans to quantitatively describe intellectual

capability in all infants and young children. Although it is sometimes useful,

as Gesell and his coworkers have done, to talk about the characteristics of the

"3-year-old" or the "4-year-old," this can be misleading.

Although some temperamental characteristics are relatively unique to each age

group, a great deal of intellectual variability exists (Gesell, 1949). This

individual variability has sometimes been obscured by the tendency to think of

young children in temperamental, rather than intellectual terms. Benjamin Bloom

has pointed out that the preschool years are a time of very rapid intellectual

growth. One characteristic of periods of very rapid growth, intellectual or

otherwise, is that they tend to exaggerate individual differences (Bloom, 1974).

Consider early adolescence and the growth spurt associated with puberty. Girls

are taller than boys of the same age, and some boys and girls mature earlier

than others. The physical variability among boys and girls in a sixth or seventh

grade classroom are incredible. In the meantime, it is critical to appreciate

that much of the variability among young children in readiness to learn has to

do with variability in growth rate and nothing more. There is a very real danger

in misdiagnosing young children as "learning disabled" when in fact

their growth is such that they temporarily fall behind their peers (Nash, 1998).

Recognizing the normal variability in growth rates is particularly important

today when the academic pressures for achievement and testing have been pushed

downward into the kindergarten and even into the prekindergarten levels

(Hoffman, 1987). One consequence of this trend is that our perception of the

range of "normality" has been compressed. The more recent view

maintains agreement with the view that normal variability is ?normal? and it

is important to give children space and allowing them to explore their own

environments. The earlier article on day-care, however, stresses the increased

need of providing all children with individualized attention and specifically

?remedial education? for youngsters from disadvantaged homes (Nash, 1998).

III. Conclusive Remarks Children of 3 and 4 years of age are unique. They are at

an age of increased intellectual growth, and the range of variability of that

growth must be recognized and appreciated in setting educational programs and

assessing educational progress. In disagreement with the special report on

?How to Make a Better Student? that emphasized reducing input, ignoring

cognitive-development tools, and giving children space, in dealing with young

children, it is well to keep in mind their tendency to think about the world in

concrete ways and to remember that their language ability often far exceeds

their cognitive understanding (Cole, 1998). The socialization of young children

is by means of frames that govern their behavior in repetitive social situations

and adults must understand when frames are spoiled, switched, or contradicted.

Young children’s emotions are simple and are expressed directly in their words

and actions. Children are most like us in their feelings and in their emotions,

and least like us in their thoughts. It is, therefore, important to treat

children with the same good manners we would accord to other adults. At the same

time, we need to remember that young children may not understand concepts the

same way we do. Put differently, we should treat young children as we might

treat a visitor from another country–with good manners, but without the

expectation that they will understand everything we have to say or be affected

from our actions even if believed to be or not be in their best interest.

7c5

Bloom. B. (1974). Stability and change in human characteristics. New York:

Wiley. Carey, S. (1989). The child as a word learner. Linguistic theory and

psychological reality. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press. Cole, W. (1998). How to Make

a Better Student. Time, 88-89. Collins, J. (1997). The Day-Care Dilemma. Time,

58-60. Gesell, A., et.al. (1949). The first five years of life. New York:

Harper. Hoffman, M. L. (1987). Empathy: Its developmental and prosocial

implications. Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln, NE: University of

Nebraska Press. Nash, J. (1997). Fertile Minds. Time, 48-56. Piaget, J. (1932).

The moral judgment of the child. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Piaget, J.

(1950). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Piaget, J. (1951). The child’s conception of the world. London: Routledge &

Kegan Paul. Piaget, J. (1952). The language and thought of the child. London:

Routledge & Kegan Paul. Tanner, J. M. (1981). Education and physical growth.

London: University of London Press.


1. Реферат Роль белорусского языка в современном белорусском обществе
2. Реферат Налог на прибыль организаций 5
3. Реферат на тему Была ли необходима Петровская модернизация
4. Реферат Синдром профессионального выгорания у медработников
5. Курсовая на тему Нормативно организационные аспекты нормотворческой деятельности П
6. Реферат Почтовая служба США
7. Реферат Трансфертное ценообразование 4
8. Контрольная работа Суицид, как форма девиантного поведения
9. Доклад Нерест жемчужных гурами
10. Реферат на тему Versilles Treaty Essay Research Paper The Treaty